Don’t like the F-word? Blame farmers and soft food. When humans switched to processed foods after the spread of agriculture, they put less wear and tear on their teeth. That changed the growth of their jaws, giving adults the overbites normal in children. Within a few thousand years, those slight overbites made it easy for people in farming cultures to fire off sounds like “f” and “v,” opening a world of new words.

The newly favored consonants, known as labiodentals, helped spur the diversification of languages in Europe and Asia at least 4000 years ago; they led to such changes as the replacement of the Proto-Indo-European patēr to Old English faeder about 1500 years ago, according to linguist and senior author Balthasar Bickel at the University of Zurich in Switzerland. The paper shows “that a cultural shift can change our biology in such a way that it affects our language,” says evolutionary morphologist Noreen Von Cramon-Taubadel of the University at Buffalo, part of the State University of New York system, who was not part of the study.

Postdocs Damián Blasi and Steven Moran in Bickel’s lab set out to test an idea proposed by the late American linguist Charles Hockett. He noted in 1985 that the languages of hunter-gatherers lacked labiodentals, and conjectured that their diet was partly responsible: Chewing gritty, fibrous foods puts force on the growing jaw bone and wears down molars. In response, the lower jaw grows larger, and the molars erupt farther and drift forward on the protruding lower jaw, so that the upper and lower teeth align. That edge-to-edge bite makes it harder to push the upper jaw forward to touch the lower lip, which is required to pronounce labiodentals. But other linguists rejected the idea, and Blasi says he, Moran, and their colleagues “expected to prove Hockett wrong.”

First, the six researchers used computer modeling to show that with an overbite, producing labiodentals takes 29% less effort than with an edge-to-edge bite. Then, they scrutinized the world’s languages and found that hunter-gatherer languages have only about one-fourth as many labiodentals as languages from farming societies. Finally, they looked at the relationships among languages, and found that labiodentals can spread quickly, so that the sounds could go from being rare to common in the 8000 years since the widespread adoption of agriculture and new food processing methods such as grinding grain into flour.

Bickel suggests that as more adults developed overbites, they accidentally began to use “f” and “v” more. In ancient India and Rome, labiodentals may have been a mark of status, signaling a softer diet and wealth, he says. Those consonants also spread through other language groups; today, they appear in 76% of Indo-European languages.

Linguist Nicholas Evans of Australian National University in Canberra finds the study’s “multimethod approach to the problem” convincing. Ian Maddieson, an emeritus linguist at the University of New Mexico in Albuquerque, isn’t sure researchers tallied the labiodentals correctly but agrees that the study shows external factors like diet can alter the sounds of speech.

The findings also suggest our facility with f-words comes at a cost. As we lost our ancestral edge-to-edge bite, “we got new sounds but maybe it wasn’t so great for us,” Moran says. “Our lower jaws are shorter, we have impacted wisdom teeth, more crowding—and cavities.”

*Correction, 15 March, 11:10 a.m.: This story erroneously stated that the newly favored consonants led to the replacement of the Latin patēr to Old English faeder about 1500 years ago. Patēr came not from Latin, but from the Proto-Indo-European language that gave rise to Latin and other languages in Europe and Asia.

  • CaptObvious@literature.cafe
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    2
    ·
    1 day ago

    It’s been a few years since Intro to Phonetics, but aren’t labiodentals produceable with either upper teeth-lower lip, as seems the case in this study, or with lower teeth-upper lip, as would be the case in edge-to-edge alignment?

    Diet affecting evolution and thus language seems plausible, but this doesn’t seem a strong aregument.

  • Lvxferre [he/him]M
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    3
    ·
    2 days ago

    The core of the research sounds convincing. Older Indo-European languages, like Proto-Italic, have a tendency to show [ɸ] instead of [f] instead - both sound similar-ish, enough to be interchangeable in almost all languages*. (Note that [ɸ] still pops up in modern languages, but it’s either as an allophone of some bilabial consonant or near rounded vowels.) Like, there is a clear pattern.

    *in fact a [f] vs. [ɸ] contrast is only attested in two languages, and both are exceptions proving the rule:

    • Ewe - [f] is pronounced with a raised upper lip, so it sounds even stronger
    • Venda - [ɸ] is actually [ɸʷ], clearly rounded

    I’m not sure if I buy the explanation that labiodentals spread because of status, however. A key difference between labiodental and bilabial fricatives is that the bilabials sound softer. It’s possible that the [ɸ β]→[f v] is encouraged because they’re easier to hear.

    A good example of that is Latin - Classical only had [f]. [v] would eventually pop up from [w]→[v] in Late Latin times, but by then any sort of “prestige” associated with labiodentals would be already gone.

    *Correction, 15 March, 11:10 a.m.: This story erroneously stated that the newly favored consonants led to the replacement of the Latin patēr to Old English faeder about 1500 years ago. Patēr came not from Latin, but from the Proto-Indo-European language that gave rise to Latin and other languages in Europe and Asia.

    They got the Latin word wrong. It’s “pater”, no macron - it has a short vowel. (At least in Latin; Proto-Italic still had it long.)

    English got that /f/ because Frotho-Germanih speahers Proto-Germanic speakers lenited most PIE *p into *f, as part of Grimm’s Law; e.g. *ph₂tḗr → *fadēr → father. Odds are that the initial sound was [ɸ].

    I also have a suspicion that what’s being currently reconstructed in PIE as *h₃ is actually [ɸ]~[β], and the reason why *b is so uncommon in the language.